Most people still believe that the world's people come divided into separate, biologically distinct groups called "races," distinguished by physical characteristics such as skin color, eye shape, and hair texture. The idea of race assumes that variation in these superficial traits correlates with other innate differences, including intelligence and aptitude - in other words, that members of one race are fundamentally more similar to each other than to members of another race.
Human beings across the world do in fact vary in their physical appearance and genetic composition. And traits like skin color, eye shape and hair texture are influenced by genes we inherit from our parents. But do patterns of human variation map onto "races"? Why do we classify the way we do, using some traits while ignoring others? Is there a logically consistent way of assigning individuals to a biological "race"?
This lesson will help students discover the fuzzy, inconsistent nature of racial classification - including how and why patterns of human variation defy racial categorization - and learn about some of the hidden values and assumptions that underlie the reasons we classify the way we do.
Students will first explore their own preconceptions about race by sorting themselves according to which race they think they belong to and listing the criteria they used. Students will then be asked to re-sort themselves according to several of their own observed genetic traits. These results will be compared within and between "races" to explore the impossibility of consistent human racial classification.
ACTIVITY #1 - Articulating Preconceptions: What Do Students Believe?
NOTE: This exercise will only work for a diverse classroom. If the class is homogeneous in terms of traits skip this activity and move on to Activity 2.
Brainstorm the different races of the world and list all the "races" across the top of the blackboard. Students might use different and inconsistent kinds of labels for their races (see below). This should not be discouraged.
Have students physically group themselves in the classroom according to the race they think they belong to. When everyone has joined a group, ask each group to generate a list of the criteria for membership in their "race" (e.g., skin color), and post the list on the blackboard under their respective racial label. NOTE: Some students may resist joining any of the designated racial groups. That's okay, but they must then add whatever label each would apply to him or herself to the list of racial labels, even "human being."
As a class, briefly note and discuss any inconsistencies across racial labels - for example, categories based on anthropology (Mongoloid, Caucasian, Negroid) vs. color (Yellow, White, Brown, Black) vs. geography (Asian, European, African) vs. ethnicity (Arab, Jew, Kurd, Hmong) vs. nationality (Mexican, Puerto Rican) vs. language (Hispanic), etc. - and similarly discuss the membership criteria listed on the board. Are they all comparable?
ACTIVITY #2: Problematizing Student Assumptions: Creating an Inventory of Traits
Screen the first five minutes of Episode 1 - The Difference Between Us, beginning with the opening titles (00:45 - 05:23; DVD Scene #2) - this is Clip A. To view a complete transcript of the episode, click here.
Stop the video and ask your students if they too believe their closest genetic matches are with others in their own racial group. As a class activity, ask your students to brainstorm definitions of "race." List each definition on the board.
Pass out the Human Traits Inventory Worksheet (below, or download PDF here), which lists several traits influenced by our genes. Have the students work in small groups to analyze their own physical traits and fill out the worksheet.
Inherited Trait |
Yes |
No |
Hitchhiker's thumb (top joint can form almost 90-degree angle) |
|
|
Tongue curl |
|
|
Detached earlobe |
|
|
Skin color - Dark |
|
|
Skin color - Medium |
|
|
Skin color - Light |
|
|
Widows peak |
|
|
Hand clasp - Right thumb on top |
|
|
Hand clasp - Left thumb on top |
|
|
Fingerprint type (click here for examples) - Loop |
|
|
Fingerprint type - Whorl |
|
|
Fingerprint type - Arch |
|
|
Fingerprint type - Tented Arch |
|
|
Blood type A (ask parent, doctor, or clinic) |
|
|
Blood type B |
|
|
Blood type AB |
|
|
Blood type O |
|
|
Lactose intolerant |
|
|
Have your students re-form their original "racial" groups. Then, proceeding down the list of traits on the worksheet, ask the students to physically re-group themselves in the classroom by each successive trait (those with hitchhiker thumb on one side of the room, those without on the other side, etc.).
After going through the entire list, discuss the following: What do the students notice about the new groupings? Do any of them match up or co-vary with their original racial groupings? How would students divide themselves now on the basis of genetic similarity and difference, taking into account all of these traits? Would it be easier to categorize people according to race if we used more traits? Why or why not?
Now, have the class re-visit the original list of membership traits for each "racial" group generated earlier (these should already be listed on the blackboard). Discuss the following: Why did they choose some traits but ignore others when dividing into racial groups? Why do we assume that traits like skin color are more meaningful than whether or not your tongue curls or whether or not your earlobes are attached? What do students think of their earlier definitions of race?
NOTE: Many students may be troubled and confused as they begin to scrutinize the "commonsense" racial categories we have all taken for granted but never examined. This cognitive dissonance is to be expected. You might re-assure students that we're not claiming that race isn't "real" (see discussions of "social race" at the end of Episode 1 and the other two episodes), but that the biological criteria we use are fuzzy, inconsistent and don't correspond to real patterns of human variation.
At this point, if you have a diverse class, you might want to re-visit the common racial skin color labels (red, yellow, white, brown). Have the class divide into groups of eight or so, and have them compare their skin colors using the inside of their upper arms (as is done in the film). What do they find? Are all "Black" people really darker than all "white" people? What about individuals from East Asian, Latino and Native American backgrounds? South Asians? What color are they really? (This can be done even with a racially homogenous class.)
ACTIVITY #3: Mapping Traits onto Racial Categories
Have students read Jared Diamond's "Race Without Color," from Discover Magazine, which includes a discussion of how various inherited traits are distributed geographically.
Working in small groups, have students make a chart that groups populations into "races" for each of the following traits (NOTE: advanced classes may also want to research these traits in other populations for a more complete picture):
Using different colored pins, have each group of students mark the geographical distribution of one of the traits on your classroom map. Alternatively, photocopy a world map and have each group mark the distribution of each of the traits on their map.
As a prelude to discussing the class's findings, ask your students to define "genotype" and "phenotype." Does phenotype always reflect genotype? What other factors might affect phenotype besides the genes? Note that with the exception of suntanning on skin color, the phenotypic traits above do closely reflect genotype. Emphasize that very few traits are influenced by only one gene. Most traits are influenced by many genes, interacting with each other and with the environment in a very complex dance that scientists have barely begun to understand.
Discuss the class's findings. What surprised students the most? Does the geographic distribution of these traits match up and co-vary with what we conventionally think of as races? Or do the traits cut across racial lines?
ACTIVITY #4: Why Racial Classification Doesn't Work - Understanding Gradual Variation, Non-Concordance and Within- vs. Between-Group Variation
We've now seen how difficult it is to find a consistent racial classification system that works for everyone. Show the following two video clips. As they watch, ask students to look for three underlying characteristics of human variation that explain why racial classification doesn't work.
To view a complete transcript of the episode, click here.
Before moving on to a discussion of why racial classification doesn't work, make certain your students understand sickle cell trait and the skin color stories told in the film. You can use the following handouts for this.
NOTE: For an excellent lab on how selective forces affect allele frequencies using the example of sickle cell, visit the Genetics Project Web site: https://gsoutreach.gs.washington.edu/instructional-materials/genetics/
Sickle Cell Handout (click to view)
Skin Color Handout (click to view)
Now, based on the film clips, ask the class to identify and describe three characteristics of human variation that would explain why it is so difficult to classify humans into racial boxes.
The three characteristics are:
ACTIVITY #5: Final Reading Assignment and Essay
As a final exercise, ask the students to read the following article about our myths and misconceptions about racial classification and its inherent subjectivity.
As a culminating activity, ask students to write an essay addressing the following questions (this essay will be used for assessment):
Consider this conventional definition of race: "The idea of race assumes humans come divided into several distinct groups, each of which carry a set of innate traits. Those who are members of one race are more genetically similar to each other, and more different from members of another race."
Is this definition scientifically sound given the results from the student activity, the film clips and the readings? Why or why not? What basic scientific criteria have to be met for this definition to work? Have your ideas about race been changed by this exercise? Explain.
Students can be evaluated based on their participation in class exercises, the thoroughness of their research and analysis, and their final essay. The essay should be judged according to how well students use evidence from the exercise, the film, and background readings to support their positions.
Should We Classify?
Following are the U.S. federal government's current definitions for the racial and ethnic groups:
Most of these categories were introduced in 1977, in response to new civil rights laws designed to remedy discrimination. Look closely at these definitions. Is everybody defined in the same way? To be categorized as Native American, for example, requires "tribal affiliation or community recognition" - a condition of no other category. The definition for African American includes a reference to "black racial groups" while none of the other categories mention race. In fact, Hispanic or Latino is defined as a "Spanish culture of origin, regardless of race." The category Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander was only introduced in 1996 - previously, it was lumped together with Asians.
What reasons might exist for defining these groups in these seemingly contradictory ways? Are the criteria social or scientific?
Even though there's no consistently objective way to classify people, can you think of reasons why we would want the government to categorize or track information on racial groups? What would happen to efforts to remedy discrimination and inequality if we didn't have any racial data?
From Mid-Continent Research for Learning and Education at http://www.mcrel.org/:
Nature of Science Standard 11 Level IV (Grades 9-12):